Table 1 Affective and Social
|
||||||||||||||
|
Importance of Problem Solving
Many employers have long regarded problem solving, critical thinking, and the ability to work on teams as critical workforce competencies (SCANS, 1991). Despite the importance of problem solving, many educational analysts and industry representatives report that students leave higher education with an underdeveloped ability to solve open-ended problems (CAHE, 2005). In part, this arises because instructors of undergraduate courses prefer students to construct knowledge through single-answer analytical problem solving before they address more complicated open-ended problems that require higher levels of knowledge (2.2.2 Elevating Knowledge from Level 1 to Level 3). Both analytical and open-ended problem-solving methods are more effective when they have process steps that are well-defined and are carried out in a systematic fashion, when there are specific strategies that prompt the practitioner to effectively carry out the steps, and when these strategies invite practitioners to take stock of strengths and weaknesses in aspects of the problem-solving process (2.3.7 Learning Processes through the Use of Methodologies).
What Constitutes a Problem?
When most students, and many faculty members, think of problem solving, they imagine a homework exercise with a single correct answer. Working out homework exercises can be considered problem solving to the extent that this activity reinforces the use of standard equations and cultivates specific problem-solving skills such as pattern recognition. However, open-ended problem solving presents a higher level of challenge because it requires the problem solver to respond to situations which are completely new to him or her (2.2.3 Developing Working Expertise (Level 4 Knowledge)). The following definition by Woods captures this essence. “The problems that we focus on to solve are ones where there is no immediately apparent procedure, idea, or routine to follow; if one has an idea how to solve ‘the problem,’ then this problem is simply an exercise. What we call a problem is a real challenge; it is a situation where we really have to struggle to define it, figure out what it means, and resolve it”.
Open-ended or creative problem-solving involves resolution of a discrepancy between one’s expectations and the reality of one’s situation; of a gap between participant expectations (future state) and participant preparation (current state). Newell and Simon stress the importance of this gap in their definition of problem solving; to them it is “a situation where a person desires to resolve a gap between a goal state and an initial state. Some blockage in the gap prevents the person from immediately seeing a course of action. If there is no blockage, then the situation is an exercise, not a problem” (Newell & Simon, 1972). Where analytical problem-solving tends to invoke cognitive skills primarily, open-ended problem-solving involves significant social and affective dimensions.
The affective and social dimensions resulting from having to address an ill-defined gap or blockage are presented in Parnes’ (1992) survey of creative problem solving. Activities such as objective finding, fact finding, problem finding, idea finding, solution finding, and acceptance finding are framed in terms of divergent and convergent questions that connect with individual and group values. Sample questions are posed in Table 1.
Table 2 Selected Methods for Problem Solving
Inspiration Method
Polya Method
Woods Method
Myrvaagnes Method
Preparation
Incubation
Inspiration
Verification
Define plan
Plan
Carry out plan
Look back
Define problem
Think about problem
Devise plan
Carry out plan
Look back
Define the problem
Identify key issues
Collect/assess information
Identify assumptions
Break problem into parts
Model Sub-problems
Integrate Solutions
Test/validate
Generalize the Solution
Communicate the Solution
Methods for Problem Solving
Many problem-solving methods are found in the literature (Woods, 2000). Four methods of varying complexity are summarized in Table 2. The inspiration method is described by Rubenstein (1975) and is attributed to Descartes. It consists of 4 steps as shown; the essential step (inspiration) is often depicted as a light bulb blinking on. Unfortunately this is how many students view the solution of open-ended problems: that only a genius can solve them. The other three methods have less ambiguous steps that are more easily conceptualized and practiced.
While the methods of Polya, Woods, and Myrvaggnes regard the first step in identifying a problem to be defining it, this is often not obvious. A problem solver may have only glimpses of symptoms and a request to “fix it.” For example, when a medical doctor performs a diagnosis, he or she observes the symptoms, reviews possible causes of these symptoms, and looks for other evidence that points toward the same possible cause until the problem appears. Even when the problem to be solved is clear, one must practice to develop skills needed to determine the usefulness of the available information, realize what information is not given, subdivide the problem, model alternative solutions, test the viability of solutions, and generalize results. As a problem solver becomes more experienced, the titles of each step in the method are sufficient to keep the process of solving the problem progressing. For novices, however, more scaffolding is needed for each step.
The benefit of Polya’s “plan” step, the “carry out plan” step, and the implied iteration in the “look back” step is supported by the fact that “many a guess has turned out to be wrong but is nevertheless useful in leading to a better one.” The “think about it” step in the Woods method (Stice, 1987) involves asking three critical-thinking questions that help to direct learning that is required to solve the problem, and actions that should be part of the plan:
What are the attributes of the problem?
What area of knowledge is involved?
What information should be collected?
Myrvaagnes (1999) further dissects the “think about it” step to include identifying key issues, assessing information, identifying assumptions, and subdividing the problem. His steps to “model sub-problems” and “integrate solutions” further explicate the “carry out plan” step. Similarly, his “test/validate” step, “generalize the solution” step, and “communicate the solution” step further inform assessment-minded thinking in the “look back” step (4.1.9 SII Method for Assessment Reporting).
What is often surprising to those observing a problem solver is the fact that obtaining the solution is not the last step. Ordinarily the relief at obtaining a solution would be cause for celebration, especially when the problem involves a new situation. However, the first solution identified is often not the best solution. For a solution to be defended as sound, it is important to validate the solution, look back, generalize, and document the solution for review by a wider audience.
Problem-Solving Skills
Problem solving is a complex performance that is built from a diverse set of cognitive and affective skills (2.5.3 Distinguishing Between Problem Solving, Design, and Research). Table 3 highlights learning skills drawn from the Cognitive Domain (2.3.4) associated with problem solving.
Woods (2000) recognizes the importance of what he calls “attitudes” that can promote or inhibit problem solving. As shown in Table 4, these are Affective Domain (2.3.6) skills that fall under self-development, emotional management, valuing self, and valuing others. To the extent that problem solving occurs in an interpersonal environment, a variety of Social Domain (2.3.5) skills, such as those outlined in Table 5, are also critical in problem solving.
Differences Between Novices and Experts
Stice (1987) and Woods (2000) suggest that successful problem solvers possess some or all of the following characteristics listed in the first column of Table 6. However, there are significant differences in the ways novices and experts manifest these characteristics.
Novices and experts differ significantly in their initial approaches to problem solving. When faced with a problem, the novice gets right to work, and is motivated not to waste time on mistakes or blind alleys. To an observer, the novice appears to be working hard. Ironically, the expert does not appear to be doing much, or making much, if any, progress. The expert rereads the problem multiple times, draws pictures and sketches, invests time building a knowledge base surrounding the problem, and explores parallel solution paths. At some point, the expert may produce a solution that appears to come out of nowhere. The two other most significant differences between the novice and expert are in their use of reflective thinking and their level of confidence in their ability to ultimately find an acceptable solution.
Concluding Thoughts
Rarely do expert problem solvers stop to share their process for problem solving. This leads many to believe that problem solving is trivial, and others to believe it is innate and magical. By making explicit the methodologies one uses, along with the diverse array of cognitive, affective, and social skills that are invoked in creating a solution, one can give others the opportunity to see the process of problem solving more clearly and more deeply. The Methodology for Creating Methodologies (2.4.16) offers advice for capturing this type of expert knowledge and promoting reflective thinking for those who are learning the process.
References
Commission on Accountability in Higher Education. (2005). Accountability for better results: A national imperative for higher education. Boulder, CO: State Higher Education Executive Officers.
Myrvaagnes, E., with Brooks, P., Carroll, S., Smith, P. D., & Wolf, P. (1999). Foundations of problem solving. Lisle, IL: Pacific Crest.
Newell, A., & Simon, H. A. (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Parnes, S. (1992). Source book for creative problem solving: A fifty-year digest of proven innovation processes. Buffalo, NY: Creative Education Foundation.
Polya, G. How to solve it (2nd ed.). NJ: Princeton University Press.
Rubinstein, M. F. (1975). Patterns of problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Secretary’s Commission on Achieving Necessary Skills (SCANS). (1991). What work requires of schools: A SCANS report for America 2000. Washington, DC: Department of Labor.
Stice, J. (1987). Developing critical thinking and problem-solving abilities: New directions for teaching and learning #30. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Woods, D. R. (2000). An evidence-based strategy for problem solving. Journal of Engineering Education, 89, 443-459.
Identifying the Problem |
Structuring the Problem |
Creating the Solution |
Improving Solutions |
Systems thinking |
Defining knowns |
Generating ideas |
Establishing criteria |
Identifying a problem |
Defining unknowns |
Applying prior knowledge |
Applying criteria to potential solutions |
Defining a problem |
Partitioning |
Selecting possible solutions |
Validating solutions |
Identifying key issues |
Organizing information |
Integrating solutions |
Assessing solution implementation |
Identifying assumptions |
Engaging in project learning |
Reusing problem solutions |
Generalizing solutions to other problems |
Identifying missing knowledge |
Prioritizing sub-problems |
Planning implementation |
Soliciting peer review |
Self Development |
Emotional Management |
Valuing Self |
Valuing Others |
Maintaining a positive attitude |
Identifying emotions |
Forming personal values |
Forming shared values |
Setting personal goals |
Expressing emotions appropriately |
Constructing an ethical code |
Committing to others |
Being open-minded |
Coping with others’ emotions |
Maintaining a sense of wonder |
Empathizing |
Persistence |
Managing stress |
Self-confidence |
Respecting |
Producing humor |
Nurturing |
Assertiveness |
Serving others |
Curiosity |
Courage |
Commitment to self |
Appreciating diversity |
Communicating |
Inviting Interaction |
Performing in a Team |
Performing in an Organization |
Reading body language |
Taking interest in others |
Goal setting |
Accepting responsibility |
Active listening |
Paraphrasing |
Achieving consensus |
Being assertive |
Responding |
Assisting others |
Planning |
Documenting |
Formatting a message |
Expressing positive nonverbals |
Cooperating |
Influencing decisions |
Checking perceptions |
Being non-judgmental |
Compromising |
Communicating decisions & results |
Identifying missing knowledge |
Prioritizing sub-problems |
Planning implementation |
Soliciting peer review |
Characteristic |
Novice |
Expert |
Initial approach to problem |
Digs into details before assessing big picture |
Reads and reflects; refines problem statement |
Prerequisite knowledge |
|
|
Goals and motivations |
|
|
Strategy |
Locks onto one solution path |
May pursue one solution path, but is prepared with one or more backup plans |
Experience transferability |
Applies experiences only in a narrow context |
Applies experiences over a broad set of contexts |
Ability to synthesize |
Sub-solutions cobbled together without synthesis |
Fluent in combining, generalizing, and simplifying ideas |